Food and Behaviour Research

Donate Log In

Sleep and Diet: Why Sleep Matters for Wellbeing and its Links with Nutrition - BOOK HEREE

Highly processed foods can be considered addictive substances based on established scientific criteria

Gearhardt A, DiFeliceantonio A (2022) Addiction   DOI: 10.1111/add.16065 

Web URL: Read this article on Wiley Online Library

Abstract:

Background

There is growing evidence that an addictive-eating phenotype may exist. There is significant debate regarding whether highly processed foods (HPFs; foods with refined carbohydrates and/or added fats) are addictive. The lack of scientifically grounded criteria to evaluate the addictive nature of HPFs has hindered the resolution of this debate.

Analysis

The most recent scientific debate regarding a substance's addictive potential centered around tobacco. In 1988, the Surgeon General issued a report identifying tobacco products as addictive based on three primary scientific criteria: their ability to (1) cause highly controlled or compulsive use, (2) cause psychoactive (i.e. mood-altering) effects via their effect on the brain and (3) reinforce behavior. Scientific advances have now identified the ability of tobacco products to (4) trigger strong urges or craving as another important indicator of addictive potential. Here, we propose that these four criteria provide scientifically valid benchmarks that can be used to evaluate the addictiveness of HPFs. Then, we review the evidence regarding whether HPFs meet each criterion. Finally, we consider the implications of labeling HPFs as addictive.

Conclusion

Highly processed foods (HPFs) can meet the criteria to be labeled as addictive substances using the standards set for tobacco products. The addictive potential of HPFs may be a key factor contributing to the high public health costs associated with a food environment dominated by cheap, accessible and heavily marketed HPFs.

 

INTRODUCTION

There is evidence that an eating phenotype exists that reflects the hallmarks of addiction (e.g. loss of control over intake, intense cravings, inability to cut down and continued use despite negative consequences) [1]. Based on meta-analyses, approximately 14% of adults and 12% of children exhibit this addictive-like eating phenotype, commonly called food addiction [2, 3]. Although some have questioned the utility of applying an addiction framework to food intake [4-7], food addiction is associated with mechanisms implicated in other addictive disorders (e.g. impulsivity, reward dysfunction and emotion dysregulation), as well as a lower quality of life and a poorer response to weight-loss treatments [1, 8, 9]. Controversy exists surrounding the role of the food in triggering this addictive-like eating phenotype. Some propose that it is the act of eating regardless of the type of food consumed that is addicting [10], or that while the type of food is important, it is impossible to classify food as addictive due to the complex nature of foods and the lack of a single addictive agent/compound [4, 5]. Food is necessary for survival and a key evolutionary pressure that has shaped reward and motivation systems across species [11, 12]. Addictive drugs that deliver high doses of reinforcing substances through rapid delivery systems tap into these systems, potently activate them and can lead to maladaptive patterns of behavior [13]. Highly processed foods (HPFs) are evolutionarily novel products made possible through modern food technology that provide refined and rapidly delivered primary reinforcers, specifically calories in the form of refined carbohydrates and added fats [1, 14-16]. The debate that remains concerns whether a refined and optimized delivery system of calories can produce comparative effects to a refined and optimized delivery system of addictive drugs.

The ability to resolve the debate about whether certain foods are addictive is hindered by a lack of identified scientifically based criteria with which to evaluate the addictiveness of certain foods. In contrast, there is a general consensus around the criteria for identifying whether someone is exhibiting an addictive phenotype [17], which has allowed for clearer criteria to guide the investigation into whether certain individuals exhibit addictive-like eating [2, 3]. There is no comparable standard for evaluating if a substance is addictive, which contributes to the conflicting explanations for why certain foods are (or are not) addictive [18].

To allow for progress on this debate, we propose a set of scientifically based criteria for the evaluation of whether certain foods are addictive. Specifically, we propose the primary criteria used to resolve one of the last major controversies over whether a substance, tobacco products, was addictive. In 1988, the Surgeon General (SG) released a report outlining scientific evidence that tobacco products were addictive. This report acknowledged that no singular criterion is sufficient to determine that a substance is addictive, and frequent (or even excessive) intake alone does not necessarily indicate addiction [19]. Rather, the SG report laid out a set of primary criteria that were identified as being necessary and sufficient to establish the addictive nature of tobacco products: (1) they trigger compulsive use, (2) they have psychoactive effects and (3) they are reinforcing [19]. A recent review confirmed that the main conclusions of this report have held up to over 30 years of scientific evaluation with some important updates based on advances in addiction science, specifically that tobacco products (4) trigger strong urges and craving [20]. The 1988 SG report was a watershed moment that bolstered the scientific recognition of tobacco products as addictive and shifted the public's perception of their addictive nature [20]. This helped to lay the groundwork for the development of new treatments that target addictive mechanisms and effective public health campaigns about the risks of tobacco products.

In this analysis, we consider the evidence that HPFs (i.e. foods with refined carbohydrates and/or added fats) [1, 14-16] can meet all the primary scientific criteria from the 1988 SG report on the addictiveness of tobacco (updated to include the ability to trigger urges and cravings). We propose that the 1988 SG report criteria used for tobacco products are ideal for evaluating the addictive nature of HPFs, as both these products are legal, easily accessible, inexpensive, lack an intoxication syndrome and are major causes of preventable death. Based on the established scientific criteria on the addictiveness of tobacco, we conclude that HPFs can meet all the criteria for an addictive substance. Below, we outline the evidence for this conclusion, highlight scientific gaps, provide a roadmap for future research and consider the implications of labeling HPFs as addictive.

PARALLELS IN THE INDUSTRIALIZATION OF FOOD AND CIGARETTE PRODUCTION

A number of terms are used to refer to foods that are prone to excessive intake (e.g. highly processed, hyperpalatable and ultra-processed), and there is a significant debate about which approach is most appropriate [21-24]. We will focus upon food categorized as HPF based on the presence of refined carbohydrates and/or added fats, as these ingredients have been most implicated in addictive-like eating [1, 14-16]. Commonly consumed HPFs include carbonated soft drinks, sweet or savory snacks, chocolate, candies, ice cream, cakes, cookies, bread and pizza (among many others) [25]. Although processed ingredients (e.g. white flour, sugar and butter) have long been available for purchase to allow for the creation of homemade HPFs (e.g. freshly baked bread and homemade cookies), a marked increase in cheap, easily accessible and heavily marketed industrialized HPFs began in the 1980s and preceded increases in obesity and diet-related disease (e.g. Type II diabetes) [26]. The dominance of industrial HPFs is the result of these products being (1) inexpensive due to the use of cheap but potent industrial substances (e.g. high fructose corn syrup an trans fats); (2) highly accessible and convenient due to additives that allow them to stay self-stable and ready-to-eat; (3) enhanced through additives that amplify somatosensory properties related to smell, taste and texture through substances not available to the home cook (e.g. flavorants, monosodium glutamate and guar gum); and (4) the focus of marketing campaigns that create positive associations and brand loyalty from a young age [25-27].

There are clear parallels between industrial HPFs and industrial tobacco products. Although dried tobacco leaves have been consumed through smoking or chewing since at least AD 800, the development of industrial cigarette-rolling machines in the 1880s revolutionized the tobacco industry [28]. Cigarettes could now be rolled at more than 10 000 a minute (compared to just a few a minute when hand-rolled). This technological shift allowed for the mass production of inexpensive industrial cigarettes and the development of the modern tobacco industry [28]. As with industrial HPFs, industrial cigarettes are convenient, accessible and heavily marketed [29]. Although hand-rolled cigarettes made from processed tobacco can still be addictive and damaging to health, it was the marked increase in cheap, convenient, accessible and heavily marketed industrial tobacco cigarettes that led to a > 1000% increase in cigarette consumption and a marked increase in smoking-related disease (e.g. lung cancer) [30]. Similarly, while home-made HPFs that include processed ingredients (e.g. sugar and fat) can be consumed addictively and damage health, the rise of cheap, accessible, convenient and heavily marketed industrial HPFs in the food supply is the major driver of excessive food intake, obesity and diet-related diseases [25-27]. In contrast, the intake of minimally processed foods (MPFs) that promote health, such as fruit, vegetables and legumes, fails to reach recommended levels despite well-funded public health campaigns to promote their consumption [31].

PRIMARY SCIENTIFIC CRITERIA FOR EVALUATING THE ADDICTIVENESS OF A SUBSTANCE

Criterion 1: compulsive use

We will now consider whether HPFs meet all criteria outlined in the SG report as necessary and sufficient to identify tobacco products as addictive. The first criterion in the SG report is the ability of the substance to trigger compulsive use, defined by ‘drug-seeking and drug-taking behavior that is driven by strong, often irresistible urges. It can persist despite a desire to quit or even repeated attempts to quit’ [19]. Intake alone is not sufficient to identify compulsive use, as only a subset (one-third) of tobacco smokers develop addictive patterns of use [32]. Compulsive use for tobacco in the SG report was demonstrated by evidence that most smokers would like to quit, but most were unable to do so. The report notes that compulsive nature is most clear in extreme cases where individuals who are experiencing significant smoking-related disease (e.g. cancer and cardiovascular disease) continue smoking [19].

It is clear that HPFs are capable of triggering compulsive use. Even in the face of significant diet-related health consequences (e.g. diabetes and cardiovascular disease), the majority of patients are unable to adhere to medically recommended dietary plans that require a reduction in HPF intake and a commonly cited obstacle for low dietary adherence is cravings for HPFs [33-37]. Failure in response to gastric bypass surgery (i.e. a significant surgery that results in the division of the stomach to treat severe obesity) provides an extreme case of compulsive HPF intake. Approximately 20–50% of individuals who undergo this surgery regain a significant amount of weight [38] and excessive HPF intake is a factor in this weight regain [38-40]. This intake persists despite HPFs triggering immediate aversive physical symptoms (e.g. cramping, vomiting and diarrhea) when consumed after surgery [38-40].

Binge eating (i.e. the experience of a loss of control when consuming a large amount of food) also provides evidence of compulsive HPF intake [41]. The type of food predominantly consumed in binges are HPFs, whereas MPFs (such as fruits and vegetables) are less likely to be consumed during binges [42-44]. Higher dietary intake of MPFs, including high-fat nuts, is inversely associated with binge eating disorder [45]. In contrast, higher dietary intake of HPFs, such as sweets and baked goods, is positively associated with binge eating disorder [45]. Similarly, rodents will risk aversive experiences (e.g. electric shock) to consume industrially produced cheesecake, pound cake, frosting and chocolate when other calorie sources (i.e. standard chow) are easily available to them [46]. Rats even show greater resistance to punishment (foot shock) when working for a ‘preferred pellet’ reward (i.e. chow with added corn syrup) than when methamphetamine is used as the reinforcer [47]. In sum, HPFs, but not MPFs, appear to meet the criterion of triggering compulsive intake consistent with addictive substances.

Criterion 2: psychoactivity

Another primary criterion outlined in the SG report is that the substance needs to cause psychoactive effects [19]. Psychoactivity was defined as ‘produces transient alterations in mood that are primarily mediated by effects in the brain’ [19]. The ability of tobacco to alter mood is more subtle than intoxicating substances, such as opioids and alcohol. However, tobacco products can cause detectable subjective increases in pleasure and reductions in negative affect [19, 48]. These mood-altering effects are related to the ability of tobacco products to deliver high doses of nicotine rapidly to the brain [19]. It is important to note that at the time of the SG report exact knowledge of how nicotine impacted on the brain, particularly in humans, was limited and mainly focused upon the ability of the drug to bind to nicotinic receptors and increase glucose utilization in the rat brain [19]. Those studies identified a host of brain regions, now known to be dense in nicotinic receptors, but of those, only the medial habenula and ventral tegmental area are established as key mediators of nicotine self-administration and use [49]. In the SG report, the ability of centrally acting nicotinic antagonist to alter tobacco smoking also provided evidence about the important role of the brain in the use of tobacco products [19].

Advances in addiction science the last 30 years have yet to lead to the discovery of a specific biomarker to identify whether or not a substance is addictive, but there is a consensus that all addictive substances increase dopamine in the striatum [50]. Relative to dopamine agonists such as amphetamine, which can increase striatal dopamine release by 1000% [51], nicotine administration causes more modest increases in dopamine efflux (150–250% over baseline), which is similar to other addictive drugs such as ethanol (also 150–200% over baseline) [51, 52]. However, despite this lower magnitude, nicotine is still capable of triggering compulsive intake and changing mood. Since the SG report, it has been demonstrated that the amount of subjective pleasure experienced in response to tobacco products is weakly associated with relapse and may be less central to its addictive nature [20]. In contrast, the ability of tobacco products to trigger strong urges and cravings appears to be a bigger mediator of addictive patterns of intake, which will be reviewed in more detail in Criterion 4 [20].

Based on the standards above, HPFs can be considered psychoactive substances. HPFs are capable of increasing positive affect and reducing negative affect [16, 53-57]. For example, intake of white chocolate and 38% cocoa chocolate are associated with ‘euphoria’ ratings on a measure of psychoactive drug effects of 7.0 and 6.4, respectively [53], which is similar to the ‘euphoria’ scores on the same measure after the administration of 1.5 mg of intravenous nicotine to smokers (score of ~6) and much higher than scores by non-smokers (~1) [48]. Intake of HPFs, relative to MPFs, is associated with greater subjective experiences of enjoyment (21.69 points higher on a visual analog scale) and the magnitude of this effect is greater for individuals with addictive-like eating [57]. Further, HPF intake is often motivated by a desire to alter mood (e.g. experience pleasure and reduce negative affect) rather than to address homeostatic needs and this tendency is associated with addictive eating [1, 58]. Further research is warranted to directly compare the magnitude of the psychoactive effects of a wider range of HPFs to known addictive substances and MPFs, but existent research supports the psychoactive nature of HPFs.

Regarding the brain, HPFs and their components increase dopamine in the striatum at a similar magnitude as nicotine when delivered orally (150–200%) [59-62]. These substances increase striatal dopamine (~150%) and dopaminergic firing rates even when oral somatosensation is bypassed and nutrients are delivered directly to the gut [63-66]. Whereas MPFs are predominantly high in either carbohydrates (e.g. fruit) or fat (e.g. nuts), HPFs are unique in their tendency to rapidly deliver a combination of refined carbohydrates and fat. The combination of refined carbohydrates (such as sugar) and fat appears to have a supra-additive effect on reward encoding in the striatum in humans [67]. Opioid antagonists are also capable of reducing HPF (including chocolate candies and cookies) intake in humans [68-71]. In animal models, opioid antagonists reduce consumption of chocolate chip cookies, but not laboratory chow [68, 72], which further highlights the importance of reward systems in the brain in driving HPF intake. As with tobacco, the experience of subjective liking of foods may be less central to their tendency to maintain compulsive intake, but rather their ability to trigger strong urges and cravings may be more central to their addictive potential [73, 74]. However, there is sufficient evidence to label HPFs as psychoactive substances based on the SG report.

Criterion 3: reinforcing

The final criterion in the SG report was that tobacco was a reinforcing substance, as defined by ‘being sufficiently rewarding to maintain self-administration’ [19]. Clearly, humans will self-administer tobacco products, although not all humans find tobacco products reinforcing [48, 75]. Nicotine was identified as a key factor in the reinforcing nature of tobacco products, as animals would self-administer nicotine, work to gain access to nicotine, prefer places where nicotine was administered and conditioned cues paired with nicotine became secondary reinforcers [19]. Compared to other addictive drugs (such as cocaine), nicotine was a relatively weak reinforcer and was only self-administered under a narrow range of conditions (e.g. intermittent reinforcement schedules, food-restriction, combined with food delivery, paired with cues) [19]. However, this level of evidence was sufficient for the SG report to conclude that tobacco products were reinforcing due to their ability to deliver nicotine.

Scientific evidence suggests that the reinforcing nature of HPFs is high. Both adults and children will self-administer HPFs (e.g. potato chips, candy and cookies) even when satiated [76, 77]. The tendency to consume MPFs when satiated appears to be lower. For example, when children are provided with post-meal access to a MPF (fruit) rather than an HPF (sweet/savory snack), energy intake is reduced by an average of 60% [78]. Both adults and children will work on an operant responding paradigm (a classic measure of reinforcement) to gain access to HPF relative to non-food reinforcers [79]. Adults, adolescents and children who find HPFs more reinforcing (as indicated by a greater willingness to work to gain access to them) are more likely to have obesity and are prone to weight gain [80-83]. In contrast, the reinforcing value of MPF (e.g. fruit, vegetables, cottage cheese) is not associated with higher body weight in adults, adolescents and children [80, 84, 85]. Daily exposure to HPFs (e.g. candy, cookies, chips) appears to sensitize the reinforcing value of these foods (as indicated by an increased willingness to work to gain access to HPFs over time) and larger portions of HPFs lead to greater sensitization [84-87]. In contrast, daily exposure to MPFs, such as vegetables and fruits, does not sensitize reinforcement and may even reduce it [85, 88]. Although more research is needed to directly compare the reinforcing nature of a wider variety of HPFs to MPFs in humans, there is preliminary evidence that HPFs have a high reinforcement value (particularly for individuals prone to obesity).

In animal models, the strength of reinforcement for HPFs relative to nicotine is very clear. The majority of animal studies that administer food as a primary reinforcer use sucrose or flavored sucrose pellets, an HPF [89, 90]. From this extensive literature, we know that animals will self-administer HPF in a much wider range of conditions than nicotine; in fact, it is often the behavior trained first before attempting to train an animal to self-administer a drug [91]. Cues presented with HPFs rapidly become secondary reinforcers and places where HPFs are administered become strongly preferred [92-94]. The ability of HPFs to rapidly deliver refined carbohydrates, fat and sweet tastes appears to play a key role in their reinforcing nature, as these factors are all highly reinforcing even when studied in isolation [95-97]. Animals will self-administer sweet tastes over cocaine more than 80% of the time [98, 99]. In contrast, animals choose to self-administer nicotine over cocaine less than 20% of the time [100]. While it is clear that animals will overwhelmingly self-administer HPFs over standard laboratory chow (which provides complete nutrition through refined ingredients) [46, 101, 102], there is a need for animal models to directly compare the reinforcing nature of HPFs to MPFs (especially those naturally high in sugar or fat—such as fruits and nuts). However, existing research highlights the high reinforcing nature of HPFs even when compared to other addictive substances.

Criterion 4: strong urges or cravings

In the 1988 SG report, craving for the substance was listed as a secondary criterion that was not considered necessary for identifying tobacco as addictive. However, the report states that the primary criterion of compulsive use ‘is driven by strong, often irresistible urges’ [19]. In the 30 years since the report, the ability of tobacco products to trigger strong, recurrent urges or cravings has been identified as a central factor underlying their addictive nature [20]. The cues paired with tobacco use and nicotine delivery quickly become salient incentive drivers of behavior [103]. Consistent with this finding, cravings in response to tobacco-associated cues are a major driver of use in humans and urges to smoke are predictive of relapse during quit attempts [20]. Further, ‘craving, or a strong desire or urge to use tobacco’ is now a diagnostic indicator of a tobacco use disorder [17]. Based on this updated science, we propose that the ability of a substance to trigger strong cravings or urges should be a primary criterion for evaluating the addictiveness of a substance.

While experiencing a strong urge or craving for high-calorie food may be adaptive when calorically depleted, craving for HPFs commonly occur even when individuals are satiated [104, 105]. The most commonly craved foods are all common HPFs (e.g. chocolate, sweets, pizza) [104, 105] and the neural substrates underpinning cravings for HPFs and other addictive substances largely overlap [106]. As with tobacco, stimuli paired with HPFs become salient motivational cues and cue-induced craving for HPFs is implicated in more frequent HPF intake, loss of control over HPF intake (e.g. binge episodes), difficulty losing weight and a failure to reduce HPF intake in the face of serious health conditions [33-37, 104, 107-109]. In contrast to HPFs, cravings for MPFs (e.g. fruits and vegetables) are less frequent and are associated with positive health outcomes (e.g. higher diet quality, longer sleep duration, successful weight loss) [105, 110-112]. Thus, there is evidence that HPFs (but not MPFs) meet the criterion of triggering strong urges or cravings in a manner consistent with an addictive substance. Future research is needed to investigate whether the intensity of cravings for HPFs is comparable to that of other addictive substances, such as tobacco.

SECONDARY CRITERIA

The SG report listed secondary criteria in their evaluation of the addictiveness of tobacco products, including withdrawal and tolerance [19]. These secondary criteria were not considered necessary, or sufficient, for determining the addictive nature of tobacco [19]. Science on tobacco addiction since the report further supports the secondary nature of withdrawal and tolerance. Many aspects of the withdrawal syndrome (e.g. difficulty concentrating and increased appetite) are only weakly related to relapse and the development of tolerance to the pleasurable effects of nicotine is limited [20]. While there is evidence of both withdrawal and tolerance in HPF intake [113-116], in parallel to the SG report, we do not include them as primary criteria.

A ROADMAP FOR FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Based on the criteria used to establish the addictive nature of tobacco, we conclude that there is sufficient evidence that HPFs can be considered addictive substances. It has been the status quo to treat industrial HPFs as food, not as the highly refined substances that they are, whose properties and components must be studied. Progress has been made identifying ingredients that amplify the effects of nicotine, such as menthol and sweeteners [117, 118]. To identify the key addictive components in HPFs, the same care and control employed in understanding the addictive potential of drugs needs to be applied to studies of HPFs.

In the case of industrial tobacco products, their complexity and inclusion of thousands of chemicals made identifying a single addictive agent challenging. In the SG report, nicotine was identified as the key addictive component in tobacco products as it was psychoactive, reinforcing and consumed compulsively [19], but at the time of the SG report (and still currently), the specific dose or absorption rate at which nicotine becomes addictive is unknown [119, 120]. The presence of nicotine alone in a product is not sufficient for it to be considered addictive. For example, some foods naturally contain low levels of nicotine but do not trigger addictive consumption (e.g. eggplant, cauliflower). Products such as nicotine patches, that deliver nicotine at lower doses and at a lower rate, also have low addictive potential. It is the ability of tobacco products such as cigarettes to deliver nicotine rapidly and in high doses that is key to their addictive potential.

Like industrial tobacco products, HPFs are complex substances that are psychoactive, highly reinforcing, st

FAB RESEARCH COMMENT:

For the related news article please see: